Welcome to the Science of Sport, where we bring you the second, third, and fourth level of analysis you will not find anywhere else.

Be it doping in sport, hot topics like Caster Semenya or Oscar Pistorius, or the dehydration myth, we try to translate the science behind sports and sports performance.

Consider a donation if you like what you see here!


Did you know?
We published The Runner's Body in May 2009. With an average 4.4/5 stars on Amazon.com, it has been receiving positive reviews from runners and non-runners alike.

Available for the Kindle and also in the traditional paper back. It will make a great gift for the runners you know, and helps support our work here on The Science of Sport.



Thursday, April 07, 2011

Specialization, training volume and talent development

Specialization, training volume and talent development

Yesterday I started what I hope is an interesting and thought-provoking series that addresses the issue of how we view and manage the process of sports talent development in young children.  I looked at a recent study of Danish elite and near-elite athletes where the authors concluded that "There is no delay in the athletic development that cannot be made up later with late specialization". 


Practice trajectory leads to performance?

That conclusion was inspired largely by their finding that elite athletes did LESS training than near-elite athletes up to the age of about 15, and then increased training time between 15 and 21.  The near-elite group, on the other hand, trained substantially more when very young, but peaked between 12 and 15, before actually dropping off.  The graph below, which I redrew using the data from the research study, shows what I called the "practice trajectory", and sums up the argument pretty nicely by showing how practice time (in hours per week) changes in the two groups


So there is a clear difference in practice trajectory, and it's tempting to say that this is linked to a difference in performance trajectory (in that they end at different levels).  However, that may not necessarily be the case, as I'll discuss below.

So today, I want to give my thoughts on the above graph, and give some possible reasons why the graph looks like it does.  I believe these potential solutions have some pretty significant implications for how we view "talent" and "hard work", and whether champions are born or made - it's unlikely to be as simple as we think!

Not specialization but training time

First, however, I have to clear up the definition of specialization and suggest a new name for the Danish study, which made a conclusion that I don't believe is supported by their data.  That study was called Late specialization: the key to success in centimeters, grams, or seconds (cgs) sports.

In my opinion, what the data suggest is that the paper should have been called "Delaying high training volume: the key to success in cgs sports.  That's because they actually found NO DIFFERENCE between elites and near-elites with respects to the training time spent on other sports - the elites did 63 months, the near-elites 62 months.

So the conclusion that delayed specialization predicts success doesn't hold up - it has little to do with specialization and a lot to do with training volume. This is important, because it's quite conceivable that a 12-year old who plays three sports (and is thus diversified) spends MORE time doing his main sport (for example, tennis) than another 12-year old who only plays tennis. Who is more specialized then? 

So, for the rest of this discussion, I'm going to leave "specialization" behind and talk rather about what the data showed, and that is that delaying higher training volumes in the selected sports predicts eventual placement in the elite group.  We need to discuss why that might be, because there are a few possible explanations.

Individual cases vs general principles

The other very important point to make is that all the data is an average (in this case, of 99 elite and 75 near-elite athletes) and within that data set, there'll be large variations.  So within the elite group represented by the beige line in the above graph, you may well have an individual who was doing 12 hours a week of training at 12, and you may also have near-elites doing less.  So we forge ahead with a discussion recognizing that we generalize, but also realizing that the question is that if you took 100 children, what would you recommend in order to maximize the chances of producing best possible performances?

Two models to explain the practice trajectory

With that, let's look at two models that might explain the practice trajectory.  To spare you scrolling up and down, here is the graph again:

 
I'd say there are three interesting questions about the changes over time in the two groups, which I've highlighted.
  1. Why do some children do more training from the age of 9 to 15?
  2. What happens in that period from 15 to 18, where there is a clear change in what the elite athletes do compared to those who will go on to become near-elite?
  3. Why do the elite athletes continue to maintain training volume while the near-elites decline further from 18 to 21?
At the end of this post, I'll come back to these three questions and summarize my answers.  So if you feel like a "short-cut", jump to the bottom and get the quick version!  For the rest, to see the arguments, read on.

The motivation vs talent model

Generally speaking, I would propose that there are two "models" that could answer all three questions at once.  There's a hybrid of the two somewhere in the middle, of course, but I'll present the extremes in the interest of clarity.  The two extremes are summarized below.


I don't have the definitive answer of which model BEST explains what was observed, but I will say upfront that I believe the answer to be a combination, but I'm leaning more towards the talent model, on the right, and I'll explain why below.


The motivational/psychological model, and the role of parents and coaches

This model holds that young children who do high training volumes early will reach a point of burnout as a result of disillusionment with the sport, and a loss of desire which is amplified by a desire to try new things.

Normally (but not always) the high training volumes early will be the result of over-enthusiastic parents, who do not necessarily intend to be this way, but who create pressure for a child to train and perform in the sport from very young.  This may take the form of overt instructions ("you will train") or more subtle cues which steer a compliant child down a focus that they might not have chosen themselves (this balance, incidentally, is enormously tricky, and it would be presumptuous of me to give advice on it, so I won't...).  There are without doubt parents who adopt the more driven approach (again, think Woods and Agassi), and it's a problem here in SA, as I imagine it is wherever in the world you read this.

Competition becomes the focus, the parent's desire to win is transposed onto a child very early, and the relative importance of success is amplified.  A coach then comes on board, perhaps with the same desire to achieve success (for the team, school or club) and this further amplifies the pressure for the young athlete.

One comment from Michael on yesterday's post put this best when he described these as "stage parents", who "are the motivator behind the child's progress.  The child is a compliant learner combined with some motivation but not much...As much as the parents provided the impetus to get [the athlete] to near-elite [levels], they are not self-driven to make it into elites" (courtesy Michael on yesterday's post, thanks!)

The consequence of all this is that at some point, the athlete turns away from the time required to train, perhaps finding other sports or stopping altogether.  This explains point 2 and point 3 on the diagram above.  I received an email from a well known SA champion runner yesterday echoing these thoughts, that the child may never have truly enjoyed the sport, but did it as a result of EXTERNAL motivation (normally parents, but also peers and coaches), which was not sustainable.  It is certainly possible, as the examples some young athletes show us, but it's the general explanation for why there may be such a high attrition rate in those who train high volumes at young ages.

On the other hand, the elite athletes are those who are self-motivated, who have intrinsic desire to train, and this is motivation that normally develops later in life.  These athletes sample sports when younger, play more and compete less, and restrain the degree of "structure" in their sport until later.  Hence, training volumes are lower initially (1), increase later when the child makes the decision to commit and train hard (2) and maintains it as they achieve success (3).  The parent, meanwhile, is probably active, but facilitate the child's motivation, allowing them to develop at their own pace (again, thanks to Michael for the description!).

In this regard, the psychology, or the loss of motivation, is the main driver for what is eventually seen as a physiological change - the athlete trains less, achieves relatively poorer results and thus seems, physiologically, to have worsened.  So psychology drives behaviour (to train or not to train), which in turn drives the physiology (and performance).

The talent-physiology model

In contrast, the talent-physiology model says the exact opposite, that the physiology drives the psychology and the resultant behaviour.  Performance is thus the ultimate consequence of physiology, not motivation.

So according to this model:
  • Some children do train harder when very young - this may be due to parental influence as described previously, or it may be because the child enjoys the sport at a young age (between 9 and 15, that is - you can go younger, but then there are other issues I won't go into here).  Whether it is parental or self-motivation does not matter too much at this stage
  • The children who train LESS at this age, and who the data suggest will go on to be elite athletes, are not necessarily the ones who are more "balanced" or diversified, but rather the ones who have a better aptitude for sport in general and thus don't NEED TO TRAIN as hard in a specific sport in order to achieve their goals at these young ages.  They often get drawn into many other sports, precisely because of some innate ability, and the result is less training in what will ultimately become their selected sport
  • In other words, their natural talent allows less training time per sport, and perhaps more 'play', whereas other children train more in a specific sport
  • An additional and absolutely crucial factor is physical maturity relative to chronological age.  Call it biological maturity, but there is no doubt that children develop very differently, and so the differences between two 15 year olds may be very large.  This has performance implications, because now you have another possibility explaining training differences up to 15 years of age, also linked to physiology:
    • Children who are early developers achieve more success at younger ages.  The faster, taller, stronger child at 14 is almost always dominant against their peers.  This encourages further training, both because parents encourage success and because children aged 14 or 15 recognize the value of success and so train more (as an aside, in SA, this is a particular problem, because a 14 year old who has developed early is big, fast and strong, and can easily be the school "hero" because of their rugby ability.  It doesn't do much for later developers, and nor does this early developer need to learn skills.  Later in life, when this advantage is eroded, he's left a little 'naked' and shown up for a lack of skill.  That's the theory, anyway...)
    • Children who are late developers remain out of competition, may train less per sport but develop skills during this time

  •  However, at around 15 or 16, these biological "gaps" start to narrow.  Late developers start to catch up, independent of training and perhaps at this stage, the 'end-result' begins to take shape.  The athletes who achieved success early on (thanks largely to early biological development) now find that their advantage is slowly being eroded.  
  • Meanwhile, the athletes who possess some genetic advantage (to use a "swear word" to those who advocate the hard-work principle for success) for the sport, such as body size, metabolic adaptations, muscular differences begin to emerge as the best athletes
  • The fact that this 'transition' in training volume happens at the age of around 15 to 17 is a very strong factor suggesting that training time may be linked to physiological development
 The ceiling comes into view

In other words, what is happening is that somewhere between the ages of 15 and 18, the "ceiling" comes into view.  You may recall yesterday that I wrote that genetics likely determines the ceiling in performance.  Training helps the athlete get there.  It's only at around 17 or 18 that it becomes more and more apparent to the athlete (and their coach and parents) that they have what it takes to succeed, or that they don't quite have it.

They may be good, and have achieved up to that point, but "great" lies beyond their capacity.  As a result, their training volume declines (2).  A loss of motivation?  Absolutely, but it's not the way the previous model defines it - here, the loss of motivation is simply because the athlete has recognized that there may not be a future in the sport.  They love it, and still do it (which is why they still train), but why do 2 hours a day to finish 20th?

At this point, the training history is irrelevant.  Those who trained less when younger did so either because they had more natural ability and so trained less or for more sport, or because they developed later and remained out of competition.  It doesn't matter, because the decision from 18 onwards is made by a athlete who is responding to their performance ability.  That accounts for Point 3 in the graph above.

Also, it's at this age that competition becomes fiercer, and so in order to remain competitive, an athlete must respond by increasing training volume, regardless of innate ability.   Their natural talent takes them to that point with less training than their peers, and at the age of 16 or 17, when most children have matured physically, they can begin to see that they have a future in the sport and must increase training volume.

Take a hypothetical case - a 15 year old rower, great as a junior because he is physically mature for his age.  But by 16, his peers are catching him as they mature.  They've been training less, but now their physiological development closes the gap on him.  Then he starts to lose, not badly, but enough that he realizes that he doesn't have what it takes.  At this point, his motivation levels decline and he trains less, the gap between him and those later developers getting larger and larger.  They have the added bonus that they have more "talent" to begin with, and pretty soon, you have this separation into the elite and near elite groups.

I think this is a crucial part of the answer - the children who train more younger probably do so partly because of parental pressure, but also because of success early on.  They do what they love, but they love it because they're good at it!  However, at some point, for various reasons, the status changes,  and the later developers, who trained less early, start to excel and then choose to train more to find further success.  So now you have a double-effect - not only do the most talented kids train more as they get older, but the less talented ones drop out and train less as they realize the ceiling is lower than initially thought.

Ultimately, in this model, it's the genetics, not the motivation, that "lets them down", because they've reached their "ceiling", which is set physiologically!  In this regard, the psychology is actually "pre-ordained" by the physiology.

Differentiating between the models - impossible, but intriguing

So which is it?  As I said upfront, I'm sure it's both.  In fact, there is evidence of reduced intrinsic motivation, and higher dropout rates, in athletes who specialize early with highly structured training.  And there are without doubt young athletes who stop training because they lose motivation and become disillusioned.  However, that doesn't control for the possibility that it's the physiology that actually determines the change in motivation over time, as I've suggested above.

And so if I have to pick an extreme, at least given the data of the Danish paper and my own insights and experiences, I'd go with the Talent-Physiology model to explain the changes in training and ultimate performance levels reached in elite athletes.

Why?  A few reasons.  First the training times in the Danish study up to the age of 15 are not really that high - an hour a day.  It's difficult to see that producing burnout, because it's actually rather low.  Three hours a day, that's another story.  So a "burnout" argument doesn't convince me.  

Secondly, and more importantly, the decision to back off and train less happens too early for it to simply be burnout from training and loss of motivation and disillusionment.  That would happen later, in my opinion, perhaps at school leaving age.  I do think that motivation levels may decline, but the main reason I would propose for a loss of motivation in someone at that age is that they stop being as successful, and decide that the investment (training time) is not worth the return (coming third or fourth or tenth)

And third, I don't think that a loss of motivation solely due to too much structured training could account for such severe reductions in training time in athletes who are still succeeding (and these athletes are).  I can think of very few athletes who retired from the sport while winning, and who did not return to it at some later stage.  I can think of many athletes who were burned out only once they started to lose - not winning is a very powerful force that strips away motivation!  So to see those changes at that young age (younger than 18), it would surprise me if it was a loss of intrinsic motivation as a result of too much structured training.  As a result of losing (the talent-physiology model), yes, but not simply because too much practice was bad for them earlier on.

Conclusion

So, three questions, and my three suggested explanations.  Again, let me emphasize this as strongly as I can - both models are plausible.  People lose motivation, and there's evidence showing reduced intrinsic motivation.  But that alone is not the driver of behaviour and less training, as I've argued.  Doesn't mean it doesn't happen, it's just that I would ascribe more cases to a talent-physiology model.

So here is the summary for those who jumped ahead!
  1. Why do some children do more training from the age of 9 to 15?

    They are either pushed by parents into training, or they choose to train in response to success in a given sport.  That success might be driven by early physical development.  Meanwhile, some children train and compete less because they're late developers, or because they simply don't need the same training volume in order to compete.

  2. What happens in that period from 15 to 18, where there is a clear change in what the elite athletes do compared to those who will go on to become near-elite?

    The ceiling comes into view.  That ceiling is determined by the genes, and some teenagers have an innate ability for a particular sport that tells them that they can "make it".  Some cannot, and they drop off in training volume, going on to become near-elites, or finding new sports altogether.  If you simply measured "intrinsic motivation" at this stage, you'd find that some teenagers have lost it, but this is not because practice is harmful, but because they aren't as successful anymore - a 16 year old is pretty savvy at picking this up!

    The fact those who did less training when younger go on to do more training now suggests that they may be developing physically to the point where they are now competitive and successful, or that they had superior ability to begin with and now respond to higher levels of competition by training more

  3. Why do the elite athletes continue to maintain training volume while the near-elites decline further from 18 to 21?

    They respond to success - they recognize that the ceiling is high, and the fact that they're now in a much more competitive environment necessitates more training.  But that is worth it, because they have the capacity to succeed at the very high level.  Their behaviour (more training) is the consequence of physiology.
Obviously, there will be debate to this point.  It contradicts what I perceive to be a general culture of saying to people that they can do whatever they wish to do, provided they train hard enough.  Log the hours, you'll earn the reward.

Unfortunately, I don't believe that sport works this way, particularly the cgs sports, because they are what I would call physiologically affected sports.  All sports are physiologically affected, of course, but these are particularly determined by physiology.  I think that the example of sprint athletes is the best I can think of to illustrate that speed is not as trainable as people sometimes suggest.

For skill based sports, maybe a different debate.  What's missing in this debate (and I'm aware of this) are some illustrations and examples of how elite athletes do progress - how do top sprinters move through childhood into adulthood?  What was Usain Bolt's rise to the top, and is it typical?  Can we find any evidence for innate vs 'earned' sporting ability, especially in these cgs sports? (Because I guarantee that there are no longitudinal studies of it!)

But that's for next time, when I'll look at 10,000 hours, and the theory of training creating expertise. 

As always, this is meant as the first word in a discussion not the last, so feel free to comment!

Ross


0 Comments: